Negotiating Errors: When the 'Adversarial Mindset' Hurts More Than Helps
Too often, the mindset in negotiating is completely adversarial—we are good, they are bad; we are right, they are wrong; we are just and they are unjust; and, perhaps most perniciously, we are reasonable and they are not and will not be so.
May 14, 2020 at 12:14 PM
5 minute read
Too often, the mindset in negotiating is completely adversarial—we are good, they are bad; we are right, they are wrong; we are just and they are unjust; and, perhaps most perniciously, we are reasonable and they are not and will not be so.
Even Ronald Reagan didn't posture in this way—his famous cry was "trust but verify." New research suggests a modification of that principle when conducting negotiations.
The American Psychological Association's "Psychology, Public Policy and Law" is publishing "The Adversarial Mindset" by Simon, Ahn, Stenstrom and Read (2020). The authors begin by surveying the literature on negotiating and identify three controlling principles:
- The "myside bias," the tendency to view a case and a position not objectively but through a more-favorable-than-justified lens. [In the world of trials, we sometimes call this "trial psychosis," a delusional belief that the case is a winner despite devastating adverse proof.] What comes with this is what the authors describe as an "unfavorable perception of one's counterpart."
- The "otherside bias," the assumption we make that our counterpart will not see things objectively but will construe the evidence and justness of the cause in a skewed manner. Accompanying this is the belief that your opponent begins from a point where they view you negatively.
- "Conflict and escalation." Where myside and otherside bias prevail, parties may tend to escalate conflict due to inaccurate perceptions rather than tend toward "de-escation through cooperative behavior."
To test these propositions, the authors conducted a series of tests, with individuals assigned to be an advocate on one or the other side in a dispute or to be the neutral third party adviser, a person directed to present the decisionmaker/arbitrator with a fair rendering of the facts that "do justice to both parties." All three roles received the same factual background, facts intended to be decidedly circumstantial and ambiguous. The dispute was over whether an employee had stolen money.
Each participant ultimately rated the strength of the facts and judged how the opponent would likely view them. Role mattered—those assigned to the employee's side viewed the facts more favorably than those assigned to represent the employer, with the neutral adviser coming down somewhere in the middle.
This is not all that was shown. Each adversary was asked to estimate how the opposing advocate viewed the evidence and overestimated how badly the opponent would view the case. Put more simply, if one party represented the employee, that person overestimated how the other side [the employer's representative] would view the proof supporting guilt.
This was but one of the two studies the authors did, with the second largely confirming the first. The concern they identify is that when we view our opponents as more judgmental and less objective, escalatory tendencies emerge.
This summary just skims the surface. The details of each experiment are revealing; and the authors frame this as proving coherence based reasoning, i.e., that the otherside and myside biases cohere to impact the judgments each adversary made. At the same time, there was some awareness of the likelihood that the impartial mediator would more fairly assess the case's strengths and weaknesses, taken by the authors as proof that there might be some self-awareness of the biases that afflict the adversaries' judgment.
What are some of the upshots? The authors note the important role of mediators in asking each side to list the weaknesses in its position, a first step toward tempering views and avoiding escalation. They also found some reason to be optimistic:
Notwithstanding the wide and deep spreading of bias throughout the participants' mental model of the case, our participants were considerably less biased when asked to assess how a neutral authority figure would view the case. In other words, our participants were cognizant of the fact that not everyone would share their view of the situation. It follows that they were to some degree aware that they were operating under the influence of bias. This partial awareness could provide an opening to bring adversaries to transcend their biased views, question their escalatory impulses and seek cooperative solutions.
For those of us who negotiate and mediate, this and similar research is critical as it shows the need to look at the case through the other party's eye and needs, and not start from a position of absolute distrust.
"The Adversarial Mindset" will be published this year in "Psychology, Public Policy and Law," https://psycnet.apa.org/PsycARTICLES/journal/law/26/1. It can also be found at https://papers.ssrn.com/sol3/papers.cfm?abstract_id=3573099.
For related research, see the March 27 New York Times article "In Negotiations, Givers Are Smarter Than Takers," explaining how giving the opposing side something that it needs may lead to more successful negotiations.
Jules Epstein is professor of law and director of advocacy programs at Temple University Beasley School of Law.
This content has been archived. It is available through our partners, LexisNexis® and Bloomberg Law.
To view this content, please continue to their sites.
Not a Lexis Subscriber?
Subscribe Now
Not a Bloomberg Law Subscriber?
Subscribe Now
NOT FOR REPRINT
© 2024 ALM Global, LLC, All Rights Reserved. Request academic re-use from www.copyright.com. All other uses, submit a request to [email protected]. For more information visit Asset & Logo Licensing.
You Might Like
View AllPa. Federal District Courts Reach Full Complement Following Latest Confirmation
The Defense Bar Is Feeling the Strain: Busy Med Mal Trial Schedules Might Be Phila.'s 'New Normal'
7 minute readFederal Judge Allows Elderly Woman's Consumer Protection Suit to Proceed Against Citizens Bank
5 minute readJudge Leaves Statute of Limitations Question in Injury Crash Suit for a Jury
4 minute readTrending Stories
- 1A Look Back at the Biggest Legal Industry Shifts in 2024
- 2Ben Brafman's Professional Legacy After 50 Years? Himself
- 3Ruling Provides Lessons for Investors: Mind Your Business (Affairs)!
- 4With SDNY Stay Lifted, Sex Trafficking Civil Suit Against Vince McMahon, WWE Gets Green Light
- 5Insurer Has No Duty to Defend 'Laidlow' Claims, NJ Supreme Court Says
Who Got The Work
Michael G. Bongiorno, Andrew Scott Dulberg and Elizabeth E. Driscoll from Wilmer Cutler Pickering Hale and Dorr have stepped in to represent Symbotic Inc., an A.I.-enabled technology platform that focuses on increasing supply chain efficiency, and other defendants in a pending shareholder derivative lawsuit. The case, filed Oct. 2 in Massachusetts District Court by the Brown Law Firm on behalf of Stephen Austen, accuses certain officers and directors of misleading investors in regard to Symbotic's potential for margin growth by failing to disclose that the company was not equipped to timely deploy its systems or manage expenses through project delays. The case, assigned to U.S. District Judge Nathaniel M. Gorton, is 1:24-cv-12522, Austen v. Cohen et al.
Who Got The Work
Edmund Polubinski and Marie Killmond of Davis Polk & Wardwell have entered appearances for data platform software development company MongoDB and other defendants in a pending shareholder derivative lawsuit. The action, filed Oct. 7 in New York Southern District Court by the Brown Law Firm, accuses the company's directors and/or officers of falsely expressing confidence in the company’s restructuring of its sales incentive plan and downplaying the severity of decreases in its upfront commitments. The case is 1:24-cv-07594, Roy v. Ittycheria et al.
Who Got The Work
Amy O. Bruchs and Kurt F. Ellison of Michael Best & Friedrich have entered appearances for Epic Systems Corp. in a pending employment discrimination lawsuit. The suit was filed Sept. 7 in Wisconsin Western District Court by Levine Eisberner LLC and Siri & Glimstad on behalf of a project manager who claims that he was wrongfully terminated after applying for a religious exemption to the defendant's COVID-19 vaccine mandate. The case, assigned to U.S. Magistrate Judge Anita Marie Boor, is 3:24-cv-00630, Secker, Nathan v. Epic Systems Corporation.
Who Got The Work
David X. Sullivan, Thomas J. Finn and Gregory A. Hall from McCarter & English have entered appearances for Sunrun Installation Services in a pending civil rights lawsuit. The complaint was filed Sept. 4 in Connecticut District Court by attorney Robert M. Berke on behalf of former employee George Edward Steins, who was arrested and charged with employing an unregistered home improvement salesperson. The complaint alleges that had Sunrun informed the Connecticut Department of Consumer Protection that the plaintiff's employment had ended in 2017 and that he no longer held Sunrun's home improvement contractor license, he would not have been hit with charges, which were dismissed in May 2024. The case, assigned to U.S. District Judge Jeffrey A. Meyer, is 3:24-cv-01423, Steins v. Sunrun, Inc. et al.
Who Got The Work
Greenberg Traurig shareholder Joshua L. Raskin has entered an appearance for boohoo.com UK Ltd. in a pending patent infringement lawsuit. The suit, filed Sept. 3 in Texas Eastern District Court by Rozier Hardt McDonough on behalf of Alto Dynamics, asserts five patents related to an online shopping platform. The case, assigned to U.S. District Judge Rodney Gilstrap, is 2:24-cv-00719, Alto Dynamics, LLC v. boohoo.com UK Limited.
Featured Firms
Law Offices of Gary Martin Hays & Associates, P.C.
(470) 294-1674
Law Offices of Mark E. Salomone
(857) 444-6468
Smith & Hassler
(713) 739-1250